last authored:
last reviewed:
invite yves talbot and melissa nutik, from UofT, to review. They presented on this at FMF 2010.
Clinical reasoning is essential to health professionals' competence, and reasoning skills should be taught and evaulated (Bowen, 2006). It is important to develop structured approaches to different chief compaints early in training, and to continue honing these approaches throught the career.
What are the most common causes? What are the most serious causes (which do you need to rule out before you go for coffee?)
reasoning process
memory: expertise and recall
knowledge representation (basic science, ; experiental knowledge)
Expert reasoning depends on extensive knowledge base, learned early during pre-clinical years, and the ability to organize and reorganize it to reach a diagnosis in a variety of ways, given presentation and context. "Clinicians often unconsiously use multiple, combined strategies to solve clinical problems, suggesting a high degree of mental flexibility and adaptability in clinical reasoning (Bowen, 2006).
Clinical reasoning requires extensive practice, and "it is emerging that central to the acquisition of expertise...is the opportunity for deliberate practice with multiple examples and feedback, both to facilitate effective transfer of basic concepts and to ensure an adequate experiental knowledge base (Norman, 2005).
The steps of clinical reasoning have been well laid out by Dr Bowen in a NEJM paper. She describesL
Chief complaint.
Initial hypothesis to guide the rest of the clinical experience.
history, physical exam, previous charts, investigations
accurate problem representation.
generation of hypothesis
search for and select the illness script
diagnosis
Junior learners are slower and require more deliberate hypothesis testing, while experts are focused and purposeful in searching for, and verifying or refuting, illness scripts during data acquisition.
Simple facts, stored and accessed from memory, plays only a limited role. Learners depend extensively on memorized knowledge, while experts use little basic science knowledge. However, knowledge representation can be very helpful.
Clinical epidemiology is also critical in clinical reasoning, and a good clinican will consider disease probabilites when attempting to apply an illness scripts.
Experiental memory plays an important role in an individual's decisions. Anchor prototypes are critical to form early in training, and a good preceptor will endeavour to ensure this happens.
Experience can also lead to idiosyncratic clinical choices, even in the face of evidence. For example, a clinician who catastrophically misdiagnosed a pulmonary embolism may order unnecessary lab tests or imaging on all patients with dysnpea, even though this is clearly inappropriate.
** Help early learners create anchor prototypes early during their training. This is important to gain early on.
Clinicians often unconsciously use multiple strategies to solve clinical problems, suggesting a high degree of (Bowen, 2006).
Come to understand, generally, what an illness script looks like.
As they progress in the capacity, learners can struggle in a variety of ways. It is important for both learner and preceptor to identify and diagnose the specific type(s) of difficulty, in order to improve. The following is modified from Bowen, 2006.
skill |
symptom |
diagnosis |
educational strategy |
data acquisition and reporting |
missing information
|
difficulty identifying or obtaining information
|
model |
problem representation |
disorganized presentation
|
lack of experience, or lack of approach |
elicit or confirm findings; link findings to your representation |
problem representation |
summary statement only loosely connected |
incomplete understanding of case; no problem representation identified |
review importance of problem representation. Ask for 1-2 sentence summary, and compare/contrast with yours |
hypothesis generation |
multiple diagnoses with no prioritization |
no problem respresentation; illness scripts not available; limited understanding of prevalence |
list important findings, formulate summary, prioritize based on discriminating characteristics for each diagnosis |
hypothesis generation |
differential diagnosis not linked with case findings |
illness scripts not yet developed, or difficulty comparing/contrasting diagnoses |
ask to support diagnoses with findings; ask for additional possibilities
|
knowledge gap |
far-fetched diagnosis |
poor understanding of cases, disease, or prevalence |
ask to describe illness script for the unlikely diagnosis, then contrast. Identify what is required to rule it out |
organization of complex information
|
disorganized presentation |
risk of premature closure when more than one problem representation is possible |
ask for plausible problem representations
|
inexperienced learner |
performance globally below expected |
has not yet committed 'anchor prototype' to memory, or too little experience |
assign patients and readings to create anchor prototype |
Can be helpful to directly observe their history and physical exam in order to validate the findings. Once this happens a few times, trust begins to build.
Skilled questioning and demonstration can be very helpful.
OSCEs can be extremely helpful. Simulated patients
Premature closure
Bowen JL. 2006. Educational strategies to promote clinical diagnostic reasoning. NEJM. 355:2217-25.
Norman GR. 2005. Research in clinical reasoning: past history and current trends. Med Education. 39:418-427.